Limits of among-group variation in each measurement
The minimum and maximum values in the among-group
variation of each measurement are shown in Table 2.
It was found here that the among-group distributions of craniofacial variables
are unimodal when the number of samples was 350 or more (in the case of
“Samples with the size of 20 or more” in Table 2), even if the skewness
and kurtosis of sample means across various Homo sapienssapiens populations were
significantly different from those of normal distributions (Table 2; Figs. 1 to
5).
The ultramodern skull of Iyeyoshi Tokugawa
(Suzuki, 1967, 1981), one of so-called aristocrats, is placed almost within the
range between the minimum and maximum values of Homo s. s. sample means, and definitely within the range between
the minimum minus 2SD and the maximum plus 2SD in main craniofacial
measurements (Table 3 and Fig. 6). But the skull of Herto [Homo
s. idaltu] (White et al., 2003) has
a very large maximum cranial length (larger than the maximum of Homo s. s. plus 4SD), though the other main
craniofacial measurements are almost the same as or smaller than the maximum of
Homo s. s. (Table 3 and Fig. 6).
Positions of sample means in within-group
multivariate distributions
Two PCAs
based on within-group correlations were first carried out as a base for
confirming the positions of the vectors of sample means in the within-group
multivariate distributions. The
correlation matrices used are of a Japanese and an Australian Aboriginal male
sample. The results of the PCAs are
shown in Tables 4 and 5, respectively.
In these analyses, the fourth variable set of the skull was used so that
the results from Homo sapienssapiens samples can be compared with the
reported data of one of the two special male specimens, Herto [Homo s. idaltu] (White et al.,
2003). The PCs obtained from the
Japanese sample (Table 4) may be interpreted as follows: PC 1 is a so-called
general size factor; PC 2, a factor associated with cranial length and height
and, simultaneously, inversely associated with upper facial and nasal heights;
PC 3, associated with nasal breadth and inversely with cranial height; PC 4,
associated with cranial breadth and inversely with nasal breadth; and PC 5,
associated with cranial length and inversely with orbital height. On the other hand, the PCs obtained from the
Australian Aboriginal sample (Table 5) may be interpreted as follows: PC 1 is a
general size factor; PC 2, a factor associated with nasal
breadth and bizygomatic breadth and inversely with upper facial height; PC 3,
associated with cranial height and length and inversely with cranial breadth
and nasal height; PC 4, associated with cranial breadth and inversely with
orbital height; and PC 5 is associated with orbital height. The repeatability of these PCs was examined
by comparing the variation patterns of factor loadings on the PCs from the two
samples (Table 6). By this comparison,
it is found that PC 2 from the Japanese sample seems to correspond to PC 3 from
the Australian Aboriginal sample, and vice versa, though the significance of
the Spearman's rank correlation coefficients obtained is not so high.
Using the
coefficients of the simultaneous linear equations for prediction of PC scores
obtained from the above two PCAs, PC score vectors for 283 sample mean vectors
from various Homo s. s. male
populations were estimated on the basis of the most reliable data (Class A in
Table 1). The PC scores are plotted in
Figs. 7 and 8 in the form of radar chart.
It must be noted here that the
axes of PC 2 and
PC 3 are reversed in Fig. 8 so that the results on Japanese (Fig. 7)
and Australian Aboriginals (Fig. 8) can easily be compared. Both Figs. 7 and 8 show that all the samples
are located between the maximum and minimum pseudo-individuals in the case of
PC 1 (general size factor), but, in the other PCs, scattered not only within
but also outside of the range. It is
also found that the PC scores for almost all of the 283 sample mean vectors are
located within the ±2SD range of within-group PC scores (Figs. 7 and 8).
Similarly,
two special specimens, i.e., the 160,000-154,000 year-old skull from Herto
(White et al., 2003) and the ultramodern skull of Iyeyoshi Tokugawa [1793-1853]
(Suzuki, 1967, 1981) were compared with the minimum and maximum pseudo-individuals
(Tables 7 and 8, and Figs. 9 and 10). Comparing
Fig. 9 with Fig. 7 (both are based on the Japanese variances/covariances), it is
found that Herto is located clearly outside of the Homo s. s. range in PC 2 and PC 5.
In the comparison with the Australian Aboriginal variances/covariances
(Figs. 8 and 10), however, Herto is located far outside of the Homo s. s. range only in PC 4. On the other hand, Iyeyoshi Tokugawa is found
to be very close to the lower limit of the Homo
s. s. range in the PC 3 scores based on the Japanese variances/covariances
(Figs. 7 and 9), while, in the PC scores based on Australian Aboriginal
variances/covariances (Figs. 8 and 10), he is close to the lower limit of the Homo s. s. range in PC 2, which corresponds
to Japanese PC 3, and, in PC 5, exceeds the upper limit of the Homo s. s. range.
Selection of samples for among-group multivariate analyses
First of all, it was checked whether or not
samples were practically usable in multivariate analyses on the basis of the
quality of samples, i.e., Classes A, B, and C in Table 1. As a result, in the combined set of the first
variable sets for the skull and limb bones, the number of male samples
available was only 5, 8, and 10 for Class A, B, and C, respectively. In the case of female samples, this check was
not made because the number of the female samples collected was too small, as
shown in Table 2.
In the combined set of the second variable
sets for the skull and limb bones, the number of male samples available was
zero, two, and three for Class A, B, and C, respectively (the check on female
samples was not made for the same reason as the above). In the combined set of the third variable
sets for the skull and limb bones, the number of male samples available was 14,
22, and 27 for Class A, B, and C, respectively.
The number of female samples available was 11 and 20 for Class B and C,
respectively (the check on Class A was not made for the same reason as the
above).
As mentioned above (also as shown in Table
2), the samples of limb bones collected was too small to carry out multivariate
analyses compared with the number of the variables to be analyzed here. Therefore, several sets of only craniofacial
measurements were also prepared for multivariate analyses to achieve the aim of
the present study.
In the first variable set of the skull, the
number of samples available was 117, 140, and 159 in males, and 38, 49, and 69
in females for Class A, B, and C, respectively.
In the second variable set of the skull, the number of male samples
available was only 5, 7, and 10 for Class A, B, and C, respectively (the check
on female samples was not made for the same reason as the above). In the third variable set of the skull, the
number of samples available was 237, 291, and 325 in males, and 42, 53, and 73
in females for Class A, B, and C, respectively.
Among-group covariations of craniofacial,
limb bone, and environmental variables
The among-group covariations between
craniofacial, limb bone, and environmental variables were also examined using
PCA. In either case of the first and
second variable sets, however, the number of samples collected was not enough
for any multivariate analysis of the three kinds of data under the statistical
restriction on sample size given the number of variables. Only in the third variable sets of
craniofacial and limb bone measurements, 27 male samples were available (in
this case, the total number of variables is 23, i.e., 9 craniofacial, 8
postcranial, and 6 environmental variables), though the quality of the samples
is not so high (Class C in Table 1). The
results of PCA and the rotated solution based on these samples are shown in
Tables 9 and 10, respectively.
It was found that PC I was significantly
associated with bizygomatic breadth and, at the same time, with humeral and
femoral midshaft thickness measurements (Table 9), and that Fac IV which was
significantly associated with average temperature was inversely associated with
absolute value of latitude (Table 10). No
association was suggested between craniofacial or limb bone measurements and
environmental variables. Incidentally,
22 of the 27 samples are those from the Japanese archipelago of the Jomon
period to modern times. This means that
the above results do not necessarily reflect a global tendency.
Among-group
covariations of craniofacial and environmental variables
Since the number of the limb bone samples
collected was too small, the among-group covariations of craniofacial
measurements and environmental variables were intensively examined. The PCA based on the data set with the
highest quality for the first variable set (Class A in Table 1) showed a very
interesting result (PC I in Table 11 or Fac I in Table 12), suggesting that
cranial breadth, upper facial height, bizygomatic breadth, orbital height, and nasal
height have a negative correlation with average temperature and a positive
correlation with absolute value of latitude.
Strangely, however, any of the factor loadings on PC I or Fac I was not
significant at the 5% level. The
significance tests were performed by Efron’s bootstrap method. Usually, the bootstrap method gives
convincing results, even if the form of distribution of the statistic to be
tested is deviated from that of a normal distribution, except for an extreme
value in an observed sample, as already stated in “Methods.” In the present study, therefore, PCA was, then,
separately applied to the correlation matrices for craniofacial measurements
and environmental variables to explore the reason
why the bootstrap method did not give an appropriate probability to a high
factor loading. The results from the
craniofacial data are shown in Tables 13 and 14,
and those from the environmental data, in Tables 15 and
16. PC I and Fac I from the
craniofacial data show that they are highly significantly associated with
cranial breadth, upper facial height, bizygomatic breadth, orbital height, and
nasal height at the 0.1% level (Tables 13 and 14). The state of high factor loadings for these
original variables is almost perfectly compatible with the results shown in
Tables 11 and 12. On the other hand, the
results obtained from the environmental data show that, although some of the
factor loadings on PC I and II or Fac I and II have very high values (Tables 15
and 16), the probabilities estimated for them by the bootstrap method are
greater than 0.05.
The same tendency as the above was
confirmed also in the analyses based on the third variable set of the skull
(Tables 17 to 22). These findings point
to a possible cause hidden in the environmental data.
In Figs. 11 to 16, the distributions of the
six environmental variables are drawn. Their
skewness and kurtosis are indicated in Table 23. They clearly show their extreme deviation
from normal distributions, except for average temperature and absolute value of
latitude. This is considered a cause of
the failure in the above bootstrap tests.
In fact, for example, the bootstrap standard deviations for the z-transformed factor loadings of upper
facial height and bizygomatic breadth on PC I in Table 11 are 1.16 and 1.40,
respectively, while those in Table 13 are 0.12 and 0.11, respectively (not
shown in tables). Namely, the former
bootstrap SDs are about ten times as large as the latter. As a result, it turns out that the former
factor loadings are not significantly different from zero, while the latter,
significantly different from zero.
In the present study, therefore, the
existence of a factor such as a PC or rotated factor was confirmed mainly by
comparing the variation patterns of factor loadings on two PCs or rotated
factors, as stated in “Methods.”
In passing, it is a noteworthy finding in
the present study that PC I from among-group correlations between craniofacial
measurements is not a so-called general size factor because of the fact that
all factor loadings on the PC do not have the same sign (Tables 13 and 19). In the case of within-group PCA, PC I is
usually a general size factor, as seen in Tables 4 and 5.
The existence of PCs or Facs obtained from
the among-group correlations between craniofacial measurements was confirmed
using Spearman's rank correlation coefficient (Tables 24 and 25). At least, the significant Spearman's rank
correlation coefficients of 0.90 and 0.98 (Table 24) between PC I’s (Tables 13
and 11) and between Fac I’s (Tables 14 and 12), respectively, suggest that
there is a common factor which strongly influence the among-group covariations
between cranial breadth, upper facial height, bizygomatic breadth, orbital
height, and nasal height. The
significant Spearman's rank correlation coefficients of 0.83 and 1.00 (Table
25) between PC I’s (Tables 19 and 17) and between Fac I (Table 20) and Fac II
(Table 18), respectively, also point to the existence of the same factor. Tables 24 and 25 furthermore suggest the
existence of other common factors.
Similarly, the repeatability of PCs or Facs
from environmental variables was also examined (Tables 26 and 27). The significant rank correlation coefficients
of 0.94 and 0.89 (Table 26) between PC I’s (Tables 15 and 11) and between Fac I’s
(Tables 16 and 12) suggest that there is a common factor which strongly associated
with average temperature and absolute value of latitude in the opposite
direction. The same tendency is also indicated
by the significant rank correlation coefficients of 1.00 and, again, 1.00
(Table 27) between PC I’s (Tables 21 and 17) and between Fac I’s (Tables 22 and
18), respectively.
Hence, PCA and the rotation of the PCs on both
craniofacial (the first variable set) and environmental variables were carried
out for female Class A samples as well (Tables 28 and 29).
The PCAs based on
Class B samples, both male and female, showed similar results to those
based on Class A samples. Therefore, the
description is omitted here.
The results of PCAs and the rotations based
on male and female Class C samples are shown in Tables 30 to 33.
Table
34 shows Spearman's rank correlation coefficients between the PCs or rotated
factors from the Class A male samples and those from the Class C male samples
of ten craniofacial and six environmental variables. These rank correlation coefficients indicate
very clear correspondence between the results from the Class A and C male
samples. It is almost true of females (Table
35). Tables 36 and 37 reveal the degree
of correspondence between males and females in Class A and Class C samples,
respectively.
Tables 38 to 47 show the results of
analyses for the third variable set of the skull, which were performed
following the same procedure as used for the first variable set of the skull (Tables
28 to 37).
Here, once again, the existence of PCs or
rotated factors from the combination of the nine craniofacial and eight
postcranial measurements and six environmental variables (Tables 9 and 10) was examined
using Spearman's rank correlation coefficients.
The results are shown in Tables 48 and 49, where, however, no clear
evidence is found for existence of concrete common factors.
In Figs. 17 to 22, the factor loadings of major
common factors, the existence of which was confirmed by Spearman's rank
correlation coefficients, are illustrated.
To know which people have the highest or lowest scores in each major factor,
standardized means of craniofacial and environmental variables were checked in
the Class A male samples with the highest or lowest
scores (Table 50). As a result, for
example, those who have the highest scores of PC I in Table 11 are peoples like
Yakuts (Russia; 73 YAKUT in Appendix 3), Buryats (Russia; 71 B-T-B) and Chukchi
(Russia; 74 CHUK3), while those who have the lowest scores of PC I in Table 11
are peoples from Lower Nubia
(Egypt; 26 L-NB2), Naqada (Egypt; 4 NAQAD) and S. Egyptians (Upper Egypt; 3
S-EGY). The standardized means of
original variables make the characteristics of extracted factors clearer, as shown
in Figs. 23 to 28.
Path analysis of craniofacial
measurements and environmental variables
Finally, path analysis was performed to get
a piece of information on some unknown factors which may influence the
among-group variations of craniofacial measurements. On a simple model, such as used in Mizoguchi
(1978, 1986), craniofacial measurements were treated as endogenous variables,
and environmental variables, as exogenous variables. The results based on the Class A male samples
of the first and third variable sets are shown in Tables 51 and 52,
respectively. In both analyses, it was
found that residual variables had relatively high values. This suggests the existence of unknown
factors influencing on craniofacial measurements in addition to the factors
found in the above PCAs.
DISCUSSION
|