First of all, it should be noted that the data collected for the present
study may contain many kinds of errors, for example, original sampling
error, intra- and inter-observer errors, errors in the determination of
chronological age, errors in the assignment of environmental data to the
samples collected, duplication of data,1) etc. Notwithstanding, the present study was
carried out in expectation of being able to get some information which are not
concealed by such errors.
Unimodal distribution of sample means and the
limits of among-group variation
It is well known that the within-group
distribution of a linear measurement of the animal body is generally a normal
distribution. And the among-group
distribution may not necessarily be normal depending on conditions. In the present study, it was found that the
distributions of sample means for craniofacial measurements across world human
populations of the Neolithic to modern times were unimodal at least in the
cases of as large number of samples as 300 or more (each sample has the size of
20 or more), as shown in Table 2 and Figs. 1 to 5. This means that the geographical/chronological distribution of Homo sapienssapiens populations is
not a uniform nor uneven distribution, and, in turn, may indicate that an
appropriate morphology of our head or face has been formed through the human
evolutionary process respondent to some selective pressure, and/or that all
recent human populations derived from a single ancestral human population. If the latter is the case, this finding may
support the Out-of-Africa hypothesis on the origins of modern humans (e.g.,
Stringer and Andrews, 2005). The
validity of these hypotheses will be discussed later on the basis of the analyses
of craniofacial measurements and environmental variables.
The limits of among-group variation of Homo s. s. in each variable are shown in
Tables 2 and 3. For major craniofacial
measurements (Table 3 and Fig. 6), at least, it was confirmed that the ranges
between the minimum and maximum values of Homo
s. s. sample means corresponded to those between the world average minus
3SD or 4SD and the world average plus 2SD or 3SD of the within-group variation
in a Japanese sample. It is also
interesting that even the ultramodern skull of a Shogun of the Edo period in Japan is
positioned within this range (Table 3 and Fig. 6). On the other hand, the deviation curve for
the Herto skull (Homo s. idaltu) is
partially outside of the range of Homo s.
s. (Fig. 6). This seems not to be
inconsistent with Figure 4b or 4c in White et al. (2003).
Comparison to
within-group multivariate distributions
How do the vectors of sample means from all
over the world distribute in a within-group multivariate space? This question was checked using two within-group
variance/covariance matrices. The
results demonstrate that most of the PC score vectors for 283 sample mean
vectors are placed within the ±2SD range of within-group PC scores (Figs. 7 and
8). This means that there are certain
constant limits in the craniofacial among-group covariations of Homo sapiens sapiens, as
anticipated. It was also clarified that there was no such a Homo s. s. population as a
pseudo-individual with the minimum or maximum values in all eight
craniofacial variables (Figs. 7 and 8).
These findings suggest some complicated system or factors controlling
the coordination between substructures of the skull (or the body). This supports Weidenreich’s (1941) idea: the
body should be considered as a totality, i.e., as a unique construction in
which all parts harmonize from the beginning of its organization, and every
essential alteration must be accounted as a consequence of a change in the
entire construction.
In addition, two special individuals, i.e. the 160,000-154,000
year-old skull from Herto [Homo s. idaltu]
(White et al., 2003) and the ultramodern skull of Iyeyoshi Tokugawa [1793-1853]
(Suzuki, 1967, 1981), were plotted in the radar charts of within-group PC scores (Figs. 9 and
10). These radar charts show that Herto
partially exceeds the upper limits of the Homo
s. s. range. This is again not
inconsistent with Figure 4b or 4c in White et al. (2003). Regarding Iyeyoshi Tokugawa, it is found that
he is very close to or slightly exceeds the upper or lower limits of the Homo s. s. range in a few PCs. This is probably due to his own living environment and genetic
changes in his family through aristocratic way of life for several generations,
i.e. a kind of artificial selection.
General size factor and regularity in
among-group variations
Usually, a so-called general size factor
(all factor loadings on this factor have the same sign) is extracted in the
form of PC I from the PCA of a within-group correlation or variance/covariance
matrix, as seen in Tables 4 and 5 or in many studies (e.g., Kanda and Kurisu,
1967; Mizoguchi, 1992; etc.). In the PCAs
of among-group correlations between craniofacial measurements in the present
study, however, such a general size factor was not extracted (Tables 13 and
19). This is robust evidence for a
qualitative difference between within-group and among-group covariations of
craniofacial measurements. Namely, this
difference is considered to result from various differences between the
ontogenetic and phylogenetic processes.
As shown in Tables
13 and 19, however, some PCs or local common factors were certainly
extracted from among-group correlation matrices of craniofacial
measurements. This fact is evidence for
the existence of some regularity in the among-group variations between craniofacial
measurements. From these tables, it is found
that, while cranial breadth, upper facial height, bizygomatic breadth, orbital
height and nasal height vary in parallel with one another, cranial length and
nasal breadth vary relatively independently of each other and of the above five
measurements. This supports Mizoguchi’s (1998b, c) findings from Asian
samples.
Česnys (1991) carried out an inter-group PCA of craniofacial measurements on the basis of 70 male Central and Eastern European
samples from the Late Mesolithic, Neolithic and Early Bronze Ages, and showed that the first PC was relatively highly
associated with bizygomatic breadth, upper facial height, orbital breadth and
nasal height, and that the second PC was relatively highly
associated with cranial breadth and, at the same time, inversely associated
with cranial length. These findings are
partially consistent with those of the present study (Tables 13 and 19).
Deviated
distributions of environmental variables
The
data of craniofacial and postcranial measurements used here have been collected
by the present author from the literature as randomly as possible in the
research environment of the present author, namely in Japan. But there are lots of bias factors affecting
the amount of data for a particular character or the shape of among-group distribution
of a variable. For example, the
geographical distribution of anthropologists (some people say that the number
of archeological sites excavated is large in the areas where there are many
universities or institutions to which archeologists belong), the research
environment of a data collector, main interest of many researchers, etc. may influence
on the amount of data or the shape of among-group distributions in addition to
a simple sampling error due to the smallness of sample size. In the present study, the number of samples collected
for limb bone measurements is smaller than that for craniofacial measurements
(Table 2). This fact may be explained by
the possibility that many anthropologists are interested in the skull more than
in limb bones.
Among the environmental variables analyzed here, only average annual
temperature has a distribution which is not significantly different from normal
distributions at the 5% level (Table 23 and Fig. 11). This may be evidence that many people prefer
a moderate climate, especially in temperature.
The
distributions of the other environmental variables are highly significantly
different from normal distributions (Table 23 and Figs. 12 to 16). In particular, chronological age of sample is
extremely deviated. This is, however,
convincing because the older the sample is, the worse the preservation of bones
is.
Regarding the deviated distributions of precipitation, relative
humidity, the absolute value of latitude, and great circle distance, various
causes can be considered. But it is not
easy to identify them without other information.
Environmental influence on the craniofacial
morphology
In his studies on within-group correlations
between craniofacial and postcranial measurements, Mizoguchi (e.g., 2007a) showed
that, for example, cranial length was associated with many postcranial
measurements. In the beginning of this
study, therefore, it was planned to carry out analyses based on the data
including postcranial measurements to clarify the causes for the among-group
correlations between craniofacial measurements.
As already stated, however, the number of samples collected for
postcranial measurements was too small to examine the global tendency in the among-group
associations between craniofacial, postcranial, and environmental variables
(Tables 9, 10, 48 and 49). Therefore, the
among-group associations between only craniofacial and environmental variables are
mainly discussed here.
Major among-group associations between
craniofacial measurements and environmental variables are shown in Figs. 17 to
22. In Figs. 17 and 18, it is clear that
cranial breadth, upper facial height, bizygomatic breadth, and nasal height
tend to be larger in colder regions of higher latitudes, and vice versa. The robustness of this tendency is confirmed by
highly significant Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients between independent
male and female samples (Figs. 17 and 18).
Crognier (1979, 1981), using “zero-order correlation
coefficients” based on 85 male samples from Europe, North Africa and
Near/Middle East, suggested that temperature (all of mean annual temperature,
mean temperature of the coldest month, and mean temperature of the hottest
month) had high inverse correlations with maximum head breadth, morphological
face height, bizygomatic breadth, and nose height. Although it is unknown whether or not
Crognier took into account the premise of bi-variate normal distribution for
correlation coefficient when estimating a correlation coefficient from the
among-group samples, the findings are very suggestive and not inconsistent with
the above results of the present study. Guglielmino-Matessi
et al. (1979) compared the PCs from 19 climatic variables on temperature and
humidity with the discriminant functions based on Howells’ (1973) craniofacial
data from 17 populations, and found that their PC I mainly associated with
temperature had significant inverse correlations with Howells’ first
discriminant function strongly associated with skull breadth and facial height
in both males and females. This is also
compatible with the findings in the present study. Gilligan and Bulbeck
(2007) showed, using male data from Australian Aboriginal tribes (the number of
tribal groups varies from 59 to 102 across the variables cited here), that
maximum head breadth was negatively associated with temperature, but
bizygomatic diameter and nasal height had no significant association with
temperature. Although their findings is
not completely consistent with the present study, this does not necessarily
mean that either of the two studies is wrong because Gilligan and Bulbeck’s
data are derived from a local region, not from all over the world. After all, the strong among-group
associations of craniofacial measurements with temperature found in the present
study are considered a result of the adaptation of local populations to their
living environments.
Incidentally,
the representative samples which are placed at both extreme positions in the
tendencies shown in Figs. 17 and 18 are listed in Table 50. The three male samples with the highest
scores of PC I in Fig. 17 are Yakuts [Russia; 73 YAKUT in Appendix 3], Buryats [Russia;
71 B-T-B] and Chukchi [Russia; 74 CHUK3], while the three male samples with the
lowest scores are Lower Nubia [Egypt; 26 L-NB2],
Naqada [Egypt; 4 NAQAD] and S. Egyptians [Upper Egypt; 3 S-EGY] (Table 50). In
Fig. 18, those with the highest scores of PC I are S.
Egyptians [Upper Egypt; 4 S-EGY in Appendix 4], Naqada [Egypt; 5 NAQAD] and Lower
Nubia [Egypt; 108 L-NB2], while those with the lowest scores are Buryats [176
BURYATM], Buryats [Russia; 175 B-T-B] and Yakuts [Russia; 177 YAKUT] (Table
50). The contrast between these samples
is shown in Figs. 23 and 24. Such
examples make our imagination real.
In Figs. 19 and 20, the variation patterns
of factor loadings are slightly different between males and females, as
indicated by Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients. But the results based on male samples, the
number of which is much larger than that of female samples, show interesting
among-group associations between craniofacial and environmental variables. Both figures show a tendency of basi-bregmatic
height and nasal breadth to be larger and, at the same time, another tendency
of minimum frontal breadth to be smaller in the regions more distant from
Ethiopia and of lower latitudes where average precipitation is higher and
average temperature is also relatively high.
Wolpoff (1968), using cranial samples from Australian
Aboriginals and Alaska Natives, showed that nasal breadth was smaller in colder
and drier regions. Noback et al. (2011),
using ten modern human population samples from five climatic groups, suggested
that nasal aperture tended to be narrower and higher in cold and dry regions,
and that the bony nasal cavity appeared mostly associated with temperature, and
the nasopharynx with humidity. According
to Crognier (1979, 1981), however, nose breadth has a significant negative
correlation with the mean precipitation of the rainiest month in males and, in
females (Crognier,1979), with the mean precipitation of the driest month. Gilligan and Bulbeck (2007), using 75 male tribal
groups of Australian Aboriginals, showed that nasal breadth is positively
associated with temperature and negatively with relative humidity. As regards the association between nasal
breadth and temperature, the finding in the present study is compatible with those
of Wolpoff, Gilligan and Bulbeck, and Noback et al. But Crognier’s results on precipitation and
Gilligan and Bulbeck’s findings on relative humidity are not completely
consistent with those of the present study.
Maddux et al. (2016) suggest that environments typically characterized
as “cold-wet” actually exhibit low absolute humidities, with values virtually
identical to cold-dry environments and significantly lower than hot-wet and
even hot-dry environments, and that strong associations between the nasal index
and absolute humidity are, potentially erroneously, predicated on individuals
from hot-dry environments possessing intermediate (mesorrhine) nasal
indices. Furthermore, Maddux et al.
(2017) state that only the internal nasal fossa, which is one of the four
morphofunctional units of the nasorespiratory tract (external pyramid, nasal
aperture, internal nasal fossa, and nasopharynx), exhibits an ecogeographic
distribution consistent with climatic adaptation, with crania from colder
and/or drier environments displaying internal nasal fossae that are longer,
taller, and narrower (especially superiorly) compared to those from hotter and
more humid environments. Although the problems
on precipitation or humidity should be investigated in more depth in the future,
it is most likely that nasal breadth has been determined through adaptation to temperature
to a considerable extent.
Regarding the within-group genetic
variation of nasal breadth, there is an interesting report. Martínez-Abadías et al. (2009), using 355 pedigree-known adult
skulls from Hallstatt, Austria, showed that the estimates of narrow-sense
heritability for cranial length, cranial breadth, basi-bregmatic height,
bizygomatic breadth, upper facial height, orbital breadth, and nasal height were
between 0.24 and 0.43, while the heritability estimate for nasal breadth was 0.00. If this can be applied to most populations,
it turns out that nasal breadth is perfectly determined by genes and
genetically extremely stable in diverse populations, such as the number of eyes
in many animals. If so, the connection
of nasal breadth with temperature woud be very strong.
Incidentally, Fabra and Demarchi (2011), using male samples from 17
pre-Hispanic populations in the Southern Cone of South America, suggested that, while nasal height
and breadth had no significant among-group association with temperature nor
with rainfall, they had significant negative correlations with altitude. The number of samples used in their analysis
is limited. But their findings are very
interesting in understanding the adaptation of the nasal structure to some
environmental factors other than temperature and humidity, such as oxygen
content.
It is also interesting here that the three
matric traits, i.e. nasal breadth, basi-bregmatic height and minimum frontal
breadth, seem to be highly associated not only with precipitation and latitude
but also with the great circle distance from Herto, Ethiopia.
Relethford (2004) comprehensively examined the
contribution of isolation by geographic distance to the present global
variation patterns in three kinds of characters, i.e., red blood cell
polymorphisms, microsatellite DNA markers and craniofacial measurements, and
maintains, as an alternative explanation, that, since a common pattern of
global gene flow mediated by geographic distance is detectable in diverse
genetic and morphological data sets, the correspondence between genetic
similarity and geographic distance reflects the history of dispersal of the
human species out of Africa. His
conclusion seems reasonable if all the geographic distances he computed are
those from Africa. In practice, however,
the distances he used contain not only distances between local populations within
Africa but also those within New World, Australasia, or Eurasia in addition to
the distances between the four regions.
Namely, it can also be said that his results may reflect the history of
dispersal of the human species out of New World, Australasia, or Eurasia. In any case, geographic distance is no doubt an
important factor in understanding the background of evolutionary processes.
As shown
by Relethford (2004), genetic similarity between populations may decrease with
geographic distance between the populations in general, and this can partly be explained
by gene flow or migration. Betti et al.
(2010) assert that neutral processes (genetic drift) have been much more
important than climate in shaping the human cranium, and that a large
proportion of the signal for natural selection comes from populations from
extremely cold regions. In both of Figs.
19 and 20 of the present study, it is found, though only in males, that the mean
values themselves of metric traits, not their differences or similarity between
populations, vary in parallel with the geographic distance from Herto,
Ethiopia. Can this phenomenon be
explained by gene flow (or migration) or genetic drift? The three male samples with the highest scores
of PC II in Fig. 19 are Chamorros [Mariana Islands; 30 MEDCHAMM in Appendix 3],
Hawaii [Hawaiian Islands; 31 HAWAI] and Dayak [Borneo; 91 DAYAK], while the three
male samples with the lowest scores are Nenets [Russia; 61 NENET], Carinthians
[S.C. Austria; 102 CARIN] and Czechs [Bohemia; 110 CZECH] (Table 50). In Fig. 20, those with the highest scores of
PC II are Dayak [Borneo; 195 DAYAK in Appendix 4], Chamorros [Mariana Islands; 115
MEDCHAMM] and Hawaii [Hawaiian Islands; 116 HAWAI], while those with the lowest
scores are Lapps [199 LAPPS], Čuden [Russia; 120 CUDEN] and Kaiserslautern
[Germany; 96 KAISR] (Table 50). The
contrast between these samples is shown in Figs. 25 and 26. If the Out-of-Africa hypothesis on the origins of modern
humans is correct, nasal breadth, for example, must decrease toward the
northeast and increase toward the southeast.
If two different critical genetic drifts happened in the process of
migration and, after that, there have been no great change in the descendant
populations, one of which migrated to a more distant region than the other,
then such a state as shown here may have appeared. But, if so, not only the above three metric
characters but also all the other characters must change in the two directions,
namely, all characters must have high correlations with geographic distance, as
stated by Fabra and Demarchi (2011). In
Figs. 19 and 20, there is no such indication.
Therefore, the strong associations of the above three metric characters with
geographic distance are also considered evidence of adaptation to environment
in each region. But it is not easy to
imagine the mechanism of adaptation for minimum frontal breadth and
basi-bregmatic height. If the
compression by temporal muscles affects the smallness of minimum frontal
breadth and the largeness of basi-bregmatic height, their associations with
geographic distance may be due to a difference between the ways of subsistence,
such as food habits, in the high and low latitudes.
In Figs. 21 and 22, it is found that
cranial length and cranial base length are highly associated with chronological
age in both males and females. The three
male samples with the highest scores of PC III in Fig. 21 are Ekven [Russia; 7
EKVEN in Appendix 3], Proto-Nordics [North Iran; 15 PN-TP] and Murray River
Valley [Australia; 11 MURRY], while the three male samples with the lowest
scores are Czechs [Bohemia; 110 CZECH], Vietnamese [89 VIETN] and Vorarlberger
[Austria; 108 VORAR] (Table 50). In
Fig. 22, those with the highest scores of PC IV are Cernica [Romania; 3 CERNC
in Appendix 4], Ekven [Russia; 9 EKVEN] and Kivutkalis [Latvia; 71 KIVUT],
while those with the lowest scores are Copts [Egypt; 144 COPTS], Armenians [231
ARMEN] and Hradek b. Mikolov [Czech; 220 HRADK] (Table 50). The contrast between these samples is shown
in Figs. 27 and 28. The time span in the
male Class A samples used here is about 7,000 years, i.e. the period of the
Neolithic Age and the succeeding times (Appendices 3 and 4). The association found here between cranial
length and chronological age, therefore, does not reflect the whole evolutionary
or dispersion process of Homo sapiens
sapiens after the event of out-of-Africa which conceivably began before 180,000
years ago (Hershkovitz et al.,
2018). Even so, however, this is an important
finding to understand the background of brachycephalization.
Causes of brachycephalization
As pointed out by Mizoguchi’s (1998b, c), cranial length and
breadth vary relatively independently of each other in the among-group multivariate
space. This is reconfirmed in the
present study (Tables 13 and 19).
Brachycepahlization or dolichocephalization (Weidenreich, 1945) is a
phenomenon associated with time. In
Japan, Suzuki (1956) reported the first evidence of brachycephalization. It is well known nowadays that dolichocephalization
proceeded from the Kofun period (the 4th to 12th century A.D.) till the Middle
Ages (the 12th to 16th c.), and then, reversely, brachycephalization started in
the Middle Ages and continued to the present (Suzuki, 1956, 1969; Nakahashi, 1987). According to Mizoguchi (1992), cranial breadth
has changed in parallel with cranial index from the Kofun period up to the
present, but cranial length has gradually and slightly decreased. (Incidentally, it is said that there is no
evidence for a great number of immigrants during the period between the Kofun and
modern times.) In a global scale, while cranial
breadth does not have any high correlation with chronological age but with
temperature (Figs. 17 and 18), cranial length has a considerably high correlation
with chronological age (Figs. 21 and 22).
From these findings, it is considered that the decrease of cranial
length in Japan is also part of such a world tendency. But the reversal from dolichocepahlization to
brachycephalization in Japan cannot be explained by the gradual change in
cranial length. Even if cranial breadth
is strongly correlated with temperature in a global scale, there seems no
evidence for a drastic change in temperature during the period between the
Kofun and modern times in Japan, as far as the present author knows.
It is
well known, on the other hand, that brachycephalization and
dolichocephalization have not necessarily proceeded at the same time or at the
same pace in various areas of the world (e.g., Ikeda, 1982; Susanne et al.,
1988; Kouchi, 1999, 2018). What is the
cause for the different patterns of fluctuation in cranial index between
geographical regions or between chronological ages?
To elucidate the causes of
brachycephalization, Mizoguchi (1992, 1994, 1995b, 1996, 1997, 1998a, d, 1999,
2000a, 2001, 2002, 2003a, b, 2004a, b, 2005, 2007a, b, 2008, 2009, 2013a)
carried out a series of PCAs of within-group correlations between cranial and
postcranial measurements on the premise that population differences are
extensions of individual differences (Howells, 1973), as mentioned above. He found several common factors suggesting
that, while cranial breadth has no consistent associations with any postcranial
measurements, cranial length is significantly associated with many postcranial
measurements, such as vertebral body size, costal chord, pelvic widths, and
limb bone lengths and thicknesses; and considered that the variation in cranial
length might, in part, be related to the degree of development of skeletal
muscles or body size and, besides, that the form of the maternal pelvic inlet might
be another important determinant of the neurocranial form. On the way to proceed his study, Mizoguchi (2004b) tentatively hypothesized as
follows: There are at least three possible causes for brachycephalization or
dolichocephalization, i.e. diachronic changes in the amount of skeletal muscles, body size (substantially the same as the amount of skeletal muscles), and the pelvic form; and, in turn, possible causes for secular changes in body size and/or in the degree of
development of skeletal muscles may be diachronic changes in quality and
quantity of available nutrition, physical activity, etc. In addition, Mizoguchi (2007b), similarly on
the basis of within-group PCAs, stated that neither cranial
length nor cranial breadth had any consistent associations with facial
measurements across sexes, contrary to our expectations, and that the difference in the
way of connecting with other characters between cranial length and breadth might
be one of the reasons why brachycephalization and dolichocephalization alternately
and irregularly occur in a geographic area.
Later, Mizoguchi (2013a), also performing PCAs of within-group
correlations, found positive associations between cranial breadth, the vertical
diameter of the femoral head (bearing body weight), nasal height (relating to oxygen
intake), and maximum pelvic breadth, and maintained that these findings were compatible with the cold adaptation hypothesis (Coon, 1962) and Ruff’s
(1991, 1993, 1994, 2002) cylindrical thermoregulatory model, in which the
pelvis tended to be wider in colder regions. Miyashita and Takahashi (1971) and Houghton (1996) already pointed out that
there were high correlations between body mass and nasal dimensions. In addition, Bastir et al. (2011) showed that males had
larger cranial airway passages, both absolutely and relatively, than females
and that males tended to have relatively taller piriform apertures, internal
nasal cavities and choanae than females, and suggested that the identified
sex-specific differences in cranial airways might be linked with sex-specific
differences in body size, composition, and energetics. These findings are not inconsistent with
those of Mizoguchi (2013a) and of the present study (Figs. 17 and 18).
In summary, it was clarified in the PCAs of
among-group correlations between craniofacial measurements and environmental
variables that people possessing the broader neurocranium and the higher and
wider face tended to live in colder regions of higher latitudes, and,
independently of this tendency, that recent people tended to have
anteroposteriorly shorter skulls than ancient people during the period of the
Neolithic to modern times. Various combinations
of these two tendencies seem to have generated the fluctuation of brachycephalization
and dolichocephaliztion in each of local regions. If the above Mizoguchi’s findings based on
within-group analyses can be utilized to explain among-group phenomena, the
decrease in cranial length may be said to have been caused, in part, by the
decrease of the body size or gracilization (Schwidetzky, 1980; Henneberg, 1988)
from the Neolithic to the present, which may, in turn, be referred to diachronic
changes of sociocultural factors, such as the development of technology, the decrease
of physical activity or labor, the improvement of nutrition (Shimada, 1974; Kouchi,
2018), the change of food habits (Mizoguchi, 1993; Noback and Harvati, 2015), the
decrease of biomechanical stress on the masticatory apparatus (Ringqvist, 1973;
Baab et al., 2010; Mizoguchi, 2012) or the nuchal planum (Mizoguchi, 2008, 2009,
2012; Zafar et al., 2000), etc. It can easily be noticed
here that the degree of sociocultural changes varies from region to region and
from times to times. Therefore, the
change only in cranial length caused by such sociaocultural factors can also
explain the fluctuation of brachycephalization and dolichocephalization in some
areas. But the same logic cannot be used
for the oscillation of cranial index oberved in Japan because the cranial index
has changed mainly in parallel with cranial breadth.
For
the present, it can be said in general that brachycephalization or
dolichocephaliztion is caused by the differential adaptations and/or
acclimatizations of cranial length and breadth to our diverse natural and sociocultural
environments and by the difference in the way of connecting with other
characters between cranial length and breadth.
Unknown factors influencing the craniofacial
morphology
Path analysis was performed as a
complementary analysis to confirm the existence of unknown factors making a
relatively large contribution to each craniofacial measurement. The results are shown in Tables 51 and
52. It was found that cranial length had
relatively high positive path coefficients on latitude, temperature, and
chronological age; minimum frontal breadth had a relatively high positive
coefficient with humidity and a relatively high negative coefficient with great
circle distance; cranial breadth had a relatively high negative coefficient
with temperature; basi-bregmatic height had relatively high positive
coefficients on temperature and great circle distance; upper facial and nasal heights
had very high negative coefficients with temperature; bizygomatic breadth and
orbital height had relatively high negative coefficients with temperature; and
nasal breadth had relatively high negative coefficients with temperature and
latitude. Namely, path coefficients from
environmental variables to each craniofacial measurement indicate almost the
same tendencies as found in the above PCAs.
In addition, the path analyses suggest that, for all the craniofacial
measurements, there are unknown factors other than the environmental variables
dealt with here. For example, the
residual variable for nasal breadth has the highest value of 0.79 of those for
the craniofacial measurements (Table 51).
In the PCA based on the same data set (Tables 11), the total variance of
nasal breadth explained by the five PCs or common factors is only 49.86%, the
lowest value of those for the variables under consideration. This means that there are some other unknown
factors which are not negligible to explain the variation of nasal breadth.
The existence of such
unknown factors are suggested also for the other craniofacial measurements. Needless to say, we must collect more data of
various environmental factors, natural and artificial (cultural, social, etc.)
and ancient and modern, to clarify the causal chain for the formation of our
morphology.
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
|